Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:48:41.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot: neuropsychological assessment and structural brain imaging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2014

David C. Bellinger
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States of America Department of Psychiatry, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Michael J. Rivkin
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States of America Department of Radiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
David DeMaso
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Cardiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Richard L. Robertson
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Christian Stopp
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Carolyn Dunbar-Masterson
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
David Wypij
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA Department of Biostatistics, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Jane W. Newburger
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, USA Department of Pediatrics, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background: Few data are available on the neuropsychological, behavioural, or structural brain imaging outcomes in adolescents who underwent corrective surgery in infancy for tetralogy of Fallot. Methods: In this single-centre cross-sectional study, we enrolled 91 adolescents (13–16 years old) with tetralogy of Fallot and 87 referent subjects. Assessments included tests of academic achievement, memory, executive functions, visual-spatial skills, attention, and social cognition, as well as brain magnetic resonance imaging. Results: Genetic abnormalities or syndromes were present in 25% of tetralogy of Fallot patients, who had markedly greater neuropsychological morbidities than did patients without a syndrome. However, even patients without a syndrome performed significantly worse than the referent group or population norms in all of the neuropsychological domains assessed. In multivariable regression in those without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, the strongest predictors of adverse late neurodevelopmental outcomes included a greater number of complications at the first operation, more total surgical complications across all operations, and occurrence of post-operative seizures. The presence of at least one abnormality on structural magnetic resonance imaging was more frequent in tetralogy of Fallot patients than the referent group (42% versus 8%). Conclusions: Adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot are at increased neurodevelopmental risk and would benefit from ongoing surveillance and educational supports even after childhood.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2014 

A dramatic increase in the number of survivors of paediatric cardiac surgery has been accompanied by recognition of their neurodevelopmental morbidities and the associated personal and societal costs.Reference Majnemer and Limperopoulos 1 The majority of patients with tetralogy of Fallot, which accounts for 8–10% of all congenital heart lesions,Reference Loffredo 2 survive to adulthood, but limited data are available on their long-term neurocognitive outcomes. They manifest deficits as early as infancy,Reference Bellinger, Wypij and duPlessis 3 , Reference Zeltser, Jarvik and Bernbaum 4 with poorer outcomes associated with a genetic/phenotypic diagnosis, pulmonary atresia, and the apolipoprotein epsilon allele.Reference Gaynor, Gerdes and Nord 5 At 4 years of age, 20% of children with repaired tetralogy of Fallot have a significant impairment in at least one neurodevelopmental domain,Reference Hovels-Gurich, Konrad and Skorzenski 6 and at school age many have difficulties with motor skills, expressive language, speech, executive control, memory, attention, and visual-motor skills.Reference Hovels-Gurich, Konrad and Skorzenski 6 Reference Shampaine, Nadelman, Rosenthal, Behrendt and Sloan 10 In adulthood, patients showed reduced IQ scoresReference Daliento, Mapelli and Russo 11 and deficits in executive functions.Reference Baker and Vorstman 12 Patients with the 22q11 microdeletion are at substantial risk of low IQ and psychiatric disorders.Reference Baker and Vorstman 12

We report here on neuropsychological and structural neuroimaging evaluations of 91 adolescents who underwent surgery in infancy for tetralogy of Fallot, comparing their outcomes to those of the general population and a local referent group.

Methods

Subjects

We enrolled patients from 2004 to 2008 at Boston Children’s Hospital. Admission criteria included age 13–16 years, diagnosis of tetralogy of Fallot with or without pulmonary atresia, and an interval of at least 3 months between the last cardiac surgery and neurodevelopmental testing. We excluded patients with disorders that would prevent completion of the study assessments – that is, metal implants – trisomy 21, or lack of reading fluency by the primary caregiver.

We recruited a group of 13–16-year-old, normally developing, referents to whom patients were compared with regard to brain magnetic resonance imaging and psychometric tests lacking national norms. The referents were not matched to the tetralogy of Fallot patients on any factors. Exclusion criteria were similar to those used in the National Institutes of Health’s magnetic resonance imaging study of normal brain development.Reference Evans 13 Referents were recruited from a variety of sources, including local paediatric practices, posted notices, and personal referral.

The study was approved by the hospital Institutional Review Board; informed consent was obtained from parents/guardians, and assent was obtained from adolescents.

Neuropsychological assessment

Intelligence

The endpoints were the five composite scores of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth EditionReference Wechsler 14 : Full-Scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory, Processing Speed.

Academic achievement

The endpoints were the Reading Composite and Math Composite scores of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-Second Edition. 15 Tables in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition Technical and Interpretive ManualReference Wechsler 16 were used to identify scores that differ significantly from the score predicted based on an individual’s Full-Scale IQ score.

Memory

The primary endpoint was the General Memory Index of the Children’s Memory Scale.Reference Cohen 17

Executive functions

A summary score was calculated by averaging scores on five subtests of the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function SystemReference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer 18 : Verbal Fluency (mean score on the letter and semantic fluency trials), Design Fluency (primary combined measure), Sorting (combined conditions score), Word Context (consecutively correct score), and Tower (total achievement score). Adolescents, parents, and teachers completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function.Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy 19 Reference Guy, Isquith and Gioia 20 The General Executive Composite score was analysed.

Visual-Spatial skills

A Visual-Spatial summary score was calculated by averaging scores on the seven subscales of the Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (non-motor) (Upper Level)-Revised.Reference Gardner 21 The copy, immediate recall, and delayed recall trials of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure were scored for Organization, Structural Elements, and Incident Elements.Reference Bernstein and Waber 22 Adolescents completed the Sense of Directions Scale.Reference Hegarty, Richardson, Montello, Lovelace and Subbiah 23

Attention

The primary score was the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Index score on a parent-completed questionnaire.Reference Conners 24

Social cognition

On the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised,Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb 25 an adolescent judged the emotions expressed in photographs of the eye region. The Autism Spectrum Quotient,Reference Wheelwright, Auyeung, Allison and Baron-Cohen 26 a self-report questionnaire, assesses autistic traits in the general population. The Toronto Alexithymia Scale,Reference Bagby, Parker and Taylor 27 a self-report questionnaire assessing emotional deficits, generates three scores: Difficulty Identifying Feelings, Difficulty Describing Feelings, and Externally Oriented Thinking.

Magnetic resonance imaging

Magnetic resonance imaging was performed on a 1.5 T General Electric Twinspeed magnetic resonance scanner at the 13.0 hardware/software configuration (General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, United States of America). Adolescents were imaged with 3D-volumetric and dual echo magnetic resonance imaging during the same scanning session. Following acquisition of the 3D volumetric T1-weighted high-resolution 3D Fourier Transform Spoiled Gradient data, high-resolution proton density and T2-weighted images were obtained. The spoiled gradient neuroanatomic data were obtained (field of view, 24 cm; contiguous slice thickness, 1.5 mm; slices, 120; repetition time/echo time, 40/4 seconds; matrix, 256×192; flip angle, 20°) in 10 minutes 20 seconds. The T2-weighted and proton density data were acquired using a dual-echo fast-spin echo pulse sequence (echo train length, 8; 3-mm skip, 3-mm interleaved; 2 acquisitions; repetition time/echo time/second echo time, 4000/14/84 seconds; 256×192; field of view, 20 cm; number of excitations, 1) in 6 minutes, 25 seconds. Whole-brain susceptibility weighted imaging data were acquired in 2 minutes, 20 seconds.

Magnetic resonance imaging scans were evaluated by a neuroradiologist (R.L.R.) blinded to the participant group. Images were assessed by visual inspection to rate the quality of data and to identify abnormalities. Abnormalities were classified by origin (i.e., acquired, developmental), type (i.e., infarction, mineralization, iron deposition, myelination delay, ventriculomegaly, abnormal T2W signal hyperintensity), extent (i.e., focal or diffuse), and anatomic location.

Genetic classification

Tetralogy of Fallot patients were assigned to the genetic/phenotypic syndrome group based on (1) medical history of a genetic/phenotypic syndrome of multiple anomalies or (2) a finding of a genetic disorder, such as 22q11 microdeletion, on formal testing.

Statistical methods

Comparisons of operative and medical risk factors, special services, cardiac symptomatology, and magnetic resonance imaging variables were made using Fisher’s exact tests for categorical measures and either two-sample t-tests for continuous measures when normally distributed or Kruskal–Wallis tests when non-normal. Comparisons with respect to NYHA class utilised Cochran-Armitage trend tests. Neuropsychological outcomes of each syndrome group were compared with expected population means when available using one-sample t-tests. Linear regression with adjustment for concurrent family social class – Hollingshead Four-Factor Index – was used to compare neuropsychological outcomes of the syndrome groups to those of the referent group when a population mean was not available – that is Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure, Sense of Direction Scale, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised, Autism Spectrum Quotient – and for such comparisons between the syndrome groups.

Linear regression analyses were used to identify predictors of neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, using the variables listed in Table 1 – except NYHA classification and any neurological event. Predictors were screened to identify associations with the outcomes having a p<0.20, adjusting for family class. Predictors meeting this criterion were included in stepwise backward analysis with a p<0.05 retention criterion. Similar procedures were used to identify factors predicting magnetic resonance imaging outcomes using logistic regression. Surgical complications included cardiac arrest, choreoathetosis, need for extracorporeal membrane oxygenation, junctional ectopic tachycardia, necrotising enterocolitis, prolonged intensive care unit course (>1 week), renal failure (serum creatinine >1.5 mg/dL), re-operation/exploration, respiratory arrest, seizures, stroke, ventricular fibrillation, ventricular tachycardia, re-intubation, pneumonia, bleeding/haemorrhage, open chest, sepsis, ventricular premature beats, supraventricular tachycardia, “TET spells” during surgery, and tamponade.

Table 1 Characteristics of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot (n=91).

DHCA=deep hypothermic circulatory arrest

* Referent group: male, 50%; Caucasian, 81%; family social class, 53±10, age at testing, 15.3±1.1 years

** Includes stroke, seizure, choreoathetosis, and meningitis

*** Score on Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status, with higher scores indicating higher social status

Results

Cardiac status and history

Of 184 patients with tetralogy of Fallot seen at our institution and who met study eligibility criteria, 39 (21%) were followed up elsewhere or lost to follow-up, and 54 (29%) declined study participation. Our study cohort included the remaining 91 patients, as well as 87 referent adolescents. Table 1 presents selected aspects of the medical histories of the patients. Most (76%) were older than 30 days at their first operation, slightly fewer than half experienced ⩾1 complications during the first surgery, and more than half had undergone ⩾2 surgeries (median number 2, range 1–7). Three-quarters (74%) had undergone ⩾1 cardiac catheterisation (median 2, range 0–14), and 32% experienced ⩾1 complications during a catheterisation. One-quarter (25%) had a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, including 22q11 (n=11), VATER/VACTERL (n=8), foetal alcohol syndrome (n=2), Alagille’s syndrome (n=1), and Turner’s syndrome (n=1). In all, 26 (29%) patients had pulmonary atresia. The syndrome and non-syndrome groups were comparable in operative measures and other medical risk factors. Those with pulmonary atresia, compared with those with pulmonary stenosis, were more likely to be neonates at the first operation (p<0.001) and to have had a first operation that was closed, that is, performed off bypass (p=0.01). They also had more total operations and catheterisations (p<0.001 each), as well as more surgical and catheterisation complications (p=0.03 and p<0.001, respectively). They were not more likely than patients without pulmonary atresia to be in the genetic/phenotypic syndrome group (p=0.80).

Among patients, medical history was significant for stroke (n=4), seizures (n=11), choreoathetosis (n=4), and meningitis (n=2). Parents reported that 19% (n=17) had been diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (16% of those without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, 26% of those with a syndrome) and 35% (n=32) with a learning disability, which was more common in those with a syndrome (13/23, 56%) than those without a syndrome (19/68, 28%; p=0.02).

Compared with referent subjects, adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot were more likely to have received any special services (82% versus 49%, p<0.001), including tutoring (52% versus 26%, p<0.001), early intervention (40% versus 1%, p<0.001), occupational therapy (44% versus 0%, p<0.001), special education (42% versus 1%, p<0.001), psychotherapy or counselling (36% versus 15%, p=0.002), and physical therapy (35% versus 19%, p=0.02). Approximately one in four (24%) patients, compared with 2% of referents, had been retained in grade (p<0.001). Receipt of neurodevelopmental services was more frequent in the group with a syndrome than in the group without a syndrome: early intervention (70% versus 29%, p=0.001), occupational therapy (65% versus 37%, p=0.03), and physical therapy (61% versus 26%, p=0.005). Reports of special services were comparable between patients with and without pulmonary atresia.

Most adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot were in NYHA class I (68%), whereas 29% were in class II, and 3% in class III, whereas nearly all referent subjects were in class I (97%), with the remaining 3% in class II (p<0.001). NYHA class was not associated with the presence of a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, but was significantly worse for the group with pulmonary atresia compared with those with pulmonary stenosis (p=0.01). With regard to cardiac symptoms, patients were more likely than referents to report exercise intolerance (24% versus 2%, p<0.001), shortness of breath with exercise (32% versus 16%, p=0.02), palpitations (20% versus 1%, p<0.001), chest pain (22% versus 3%, p<0.001), and general fatigue (24% versus 1%, p<0.001). Reports of cardiac symptoms were similar between syndrome groups. Patients with pulmonary atresia were more likely than patients without pulmonary atresia to report shortness of breath with exercise (54% versus 23%, p=0.006). There were three patients (3%) who had a pacemaker. Medications included angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors (n=4), β-blockers (n=5), digoxin (n=8), diuretics (n=4), medication for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (n=10), and psychotropic medications (n=5). There were two adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot who reported use of both attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and psychotropic medications.

Neuropsychological assessment

The mean age (standard deviation) of the patients was 14.6 (1.2) years. Table 2 summarises the test scores of all patients and scores stratified by the presence of a genetic/phenotypic syndrome.

Table 2 Neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot according to genetic diagnosis.

* Determined by linear regression with adjustment for concurrent family social status

** Determined by one-sample t-tests comparing the tetralogy of Fallot group – with or without genetic diagnosis – with expected population means of 100, 10, or 50, as appropriate

*** Expected population means are not available; p-value is determined by linear regression comparing the tetralogy of Fallot group – with or without genetic diagnosis – with a group of 87 referents, with adjustment for concurrent family social status. The scores (mean±standard deviation) of the referents were as follows:

Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure

Copy: Organisation 10.8±2.6, Structural Element: 24.9±0.5, Incidental Element: 38.8±0.6

Immediate Recall: Organisation: 8.9±3.8, Structural Element: 23.4±3.2, Incidental Element: 28.9±6.6

Delayed Recall: Organisation: 8.7±3.5, Structural Element: 23.8±3.5, Incidental Element: 29.0±6.5

Sense of Direction Scale: 50.2±10.2

Read the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised: 23.5±4.7

Autism Spectrum Quotient: 14.9±5.9

Missing <10% of outcomes except for Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Teacher (n=58), Reading the Mind in the Eyes (n=66) and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (n=27) tasks.

General intelligence

Scores of patients without a genetic/phenotype diagnosis were lower than the expected population means for each index from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition except Verbal Comprehension, with differences of 6–10 points (0.4–0.7 standard deviation). Almost a third of patients (31%) had a Full-Scale IQ score of ⩽85 (compared with an expected frequency of 16%) and 16% had a score ⩽70 (compared with an expected 2%).

Scores of adolescents with a syndrome were significantly lower than those of adolescents without a syndrome on all composites, with differences approaching, and in some cases exceeding, one standard deviation. Half of these adolescents had a Full-Scale IQ score ⩽85, and 36% a score ⩽70.

Academic achievement

The Reading and Math Composite scores of adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic diagnosis were slightly lower, though not significantly, than the expected population means. On the Reading Composite, 21% scored ⩽85 and 6% scored ⩽70. On the Math Composite, 29% scored ⩽85 and 19% scored ⩽70. The frequencies of scores significantly lower than expected based on Full-Scale IQ score were 23% for Reading Composite and 29% for Math Composite.

Reading Composite and Mathematics Composite scores of adolescents with a syndrome were significantly lower than scores of adolescents without a syndrome, with the deficit being substantially greater on mathematics than reading. Compared with the expected population, the average deficit on the Mathematics Composite score for those with a syndrome was 27 points (1.8 standard deviation). Half (52%) scored ⩽85 on the Reading Composite and 26% scored ⩽70. The corresponding figures for the Math Composite were 57% and 48%, respectively. On the Reading Composite, 18% scored significantly lower than would be expected based on Full-Scale IQ, and 55% scored significantly lower on the Math Composite.

Memory

The mean General Memory Index score of adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic diagnosis was ~0.8 standard deviation lower than the expected population mean. Almost half (47%) scored ⩽85 and 20% scored ⩽70. In general, verbal memory scores exceeded visual memory scores, for both immediate and delayed trials.

The scores of adolescents with a genetic/phenotypic syndrome tended to be lower than those without a syndrome, with a mean difference of approximately two-thirds of a standard deviation. The mean score was ~1.4 standard deviations lower than the expected population mean; 57% of adolescents scored ⩽85 and 38% scored ⩽70.

Executive Functions

The Executive Function Summary scores of adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome were significantly lower than the expected population mean of 10; 22% scored ⩾1 standard deviation (i.e., 3 points) below the expected mean, and 7% scored ⩾2 standard deviations or below the expected mean. The mean score of adolescents with a syndrome was significantly lower than that of adolescents without a syndrome; 48% scored ⩾1 standard deviation below the expected mean, and 29% scored ⩾2 standard deviations or more below.

On the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, both parent- and teacher-reported General Executive Composite scores were higher – that is, more problematic – than the expected population mean. For adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome, teachers tended to report more problems than parents. On the parent questionnaire, 27% of adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome had a score⩾65 – “of potential clinical significance” – compared with 48% of adolescents with a syndrome. On the teacher questionnaire, these figures were 36% and 50%, respectively. On the adolescent self-report questionnaire, those without and with a syndrome scored similarly – 12% and 11% of scores, respectively, were in the range of potential clinical significance. Remarkably, neither group differed significantly from the expected population mean.

Visual-spatial skills

Patients scored significantly lower than the expected population mean on the Visual-Spatial Summary score, with the difference corresponding to 1 standard deviation for adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic diagnosis and 2 standard deviation for adolescents with a genetic/phenotypic syndrome. On the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure, adolescents with a syndrome scored significantly lower than those without a syndrome on all three endpoints for each condition – that is, Copy, Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall.

On the Sense of Direction Scale, scores of adolescents in the two syndrome groups did not differ significantly, nor did the scores of either group differ significantly from those of the referent group.

Attention

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Index scores of the adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome were ~0.75 standard deviation higher – that is less optimal – than the expected population mean. Approximately one in five (21%) scored ⩾66, which is considered to be “moderately atypical”.Reference Conners 24

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Index scores of the adolescents with a syndrome were significantly higher than those of adolescents without a syndrome, with the difference in means being 0.8 standard deviation, and ~1.5 standard deviation higher than the expected population mean. Almost half (48%) scored ⩾66.

Social cognition

On the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised, scores of the syndrome groups did not differ significantly, but both groups scored significantly lower (worse) than the referent group. This pattern was also evident for scores on the Autism Spectrum Quotient, with adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot scoring significantly higher (worse) than referents. Scores on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale were similar in the two syndrome groups, but the adolescents with a syndrome scored significantly higher (worse) than referents on the Difficulty Identifying Feelings and Difficulty Describing Feelings subscales (p=0.01, 0.04, respectively).

Comparison of tetralogy of Fallot subgroups with and without pulmonary atresia

Patients with pulmonary atresia consistently performed worse than those without pulmonary atresia, with many of the relative deficits approaching, and in some cases exceeding, 0.5 standard deviation in magnitude (data not shown). For most endpoints, however, the difference was not statistically significant, presumably because of the small size of the pulmonary atresia subgroup.

Magnetic resonance imaging

Complete data were available for 66 adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot and 75 referents. The frequency of “any abnormality” was fivefold greater among the patients than the referents (42% versus 8%, p<0.001; Table 3). The great majority of abnormalities were focal or multifocal, with most involving brain mineralisation or iron deposits. Developmental abnormalities were almost twice as common among patients as referents (9% versus 5%, respectively), but the difference did not reach statistical significance, with limited statistical power.

Table 3 Structural magnetic resonance imaging findings of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot and referents.

TOF=tetralogy of Fallot

* Determined by Fisher’s exact tests

** The major malformation is hypoplasia in the right cerebellar hemisphere and Dandy–Walker spectrum anomaly

*** Minor malformations include agenesis of the corpus and partial anterior absence of corpus callosum – no other findings of holoprosencephaly – arachnoid cyst, subependymal nodular heteropia (right parietal white matter), absent splenium of corpus callosum (not agenesis), and mildly dysmorphic ventricles in TOF group; and Chiari malformation and developmental venous anomaly in right parietal lobe in referent group

The patients with and without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome did not differ in the frequency of abnormalities. The frequency of any abnormality was greater (p<0.001) among patients with pulmonary atresia (13/16, 81%) than those without pulmonary atresia (15/50, 30%).

Predictors of neuropsychological and magnetic resonance imaging outcomes

Table 4 lists potential predictors that were retained in stepwise linear regression analyses of selected neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents without a genetic/phenotypic syndrome. The number of complications at first operation was associated with the greatest number of scores. For many scores, a “dose-related” increase in the deficit was observed. Compared with adolescents with no complications, those with ⩾2 complications scored an average of 17 points lower in Verbal Comprehension, and adolescents with one complication averaged 7 points lower. Similarly, adolescents with ⩾2 complications scored 29 points lower on the Mathematics Composite and 25 points lower on the Visual-Spatial Summary score, whereas those with one complication scored 11 and 13 points lower, respectively. Other covariates associated with multiple endpoints were the total number of surgical complications, gestational age, and seizures.

Table 4 Stepwise linear regression of select neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot without genetic anomalies (n=68), adjusting for concurrent family social status.

ADHD=Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; DHCA=deep hypothermic circulatory arrest

All characteristics from Table 1 were considered for inclusion in the regression models except NYHA classification and any neurological event

Coefficients for intercepts and family social status are not reported

In analyses of three magnetic resonance imaging endpoints – that is, any abnormality, focal/multifocal abnormality, brain mineralisation/iron deposit – patients with pulmonary atresia had greater odds of any abnormality (odds ratio=10.1, 95% confidence interval: 2.5–40.7) or a brain mineralisation deposit (odds ratio=4.1, 95% confidence interval: 1.2–13.3) (Table 5). Reduced odds of focal/multifocal abnormality were significantly associated with higher birth weight (odds ratio=0.11, 95% confidence interval: 0.01–0.87). The confidence intervals for all of these odds ratios are wide because of small cell counts, however.

Table 5 Stepwise logistic regression of select magnetic resonance imaging outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot (n=66).

Values are estimated odds ratios, 95% confidence intervals (CI), and p-values

All characteristics from Table 1 were considered for inclusion in the regression models except NYHA classification and any neurological event

Discussion

We found that a substantial proportion of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot were experiencing difficulty meeting neurodevelopmental expectations, with many receiving remedial services, having been retained in grade, and manifesting deficits, compared with normative populations. Areas of diminished performance included general intelligence, academic achievement, memory, executive functions, visual-spatial skills, attention, and social cognition. These findings suggest that the deficits observed in patients at younger agesReference Bellinger, Wypij and duPlessis 3 Reference Miatton, De Wolf, François, Thiery and Vingerhoets 9 persist into adolescence. Because adolescent neurocognitive and behavioural function strongly predicts adult functioning, our data suggest the need to develop strategies to optimise outcomes of the growing population of adults with repaired tetralogy of Fallot.

Among patients without known genetic/phenotypic diagnoses, medical factors that had the strongest associations with neurodevelopmental outcomes included a greater number of complications at the first surgery, more total surgical complications across all operations, and occurrence of post-operative seizures. The number of complications at the first operation was “dose related” with many endpoints, as adolescents with two or more complications scored almost 2 standard deviations lower on some tests than patients with no complications. Adolescents who experienced post-operative seizures were more likely to be rated as having difficulties with executive functions and attention. For magnetic resonance imaging outcomes, the diagnosis of pulmonary atresia was associated with a greater likelihood of at least one structural abnormality.

In most respects, patients with a genetic/phenotypic diagnosis performed significantly worse than patients without such a diagnosis, confirming the findings of Zeltser et alReference Zeltser, Jarvik and Bernbaum 4 and others, though because of small numbers we were not able to discern diagnosis-specific associations. In addition to the well-known relationship of tetralogy of Fallot to velo-cardio-facial syndrome, most often related to 22q11 microdeletion, tetralogy of Fallot is also associated with gene mutation or deletion – for example, NKX2.5, JAG1, TBX5, TBX1, and FOXC2. Indeed, molecular genetic analyses have defined a genetic aetiology in >20% of patients – single gene defects and/or cytogenetic abnormalities – and unidentified monogenic mutations may account for an even greater proportion of cases. We hypothesise that as-yet unknown genetic factors involved in the aetiology of tetralogy of Fallot may also impact brain development and could explain, in part, the observation that even patients without known syndromes exhibited significant deficits.

With regard to executive functions, adolescents rated their skills as being average, but both parents and teachers identified many weaknesses, and the frequencies of scores of clinical concern were several-fold greater for these informants than for the adolescents themselves. Similar findings have been reported in adolescents with d-transposition of the great arteries,Reference Bellinger, Wypij and Rivkin 28 suggesting that relying solely on self-reports of patients with congenital heart disease might underestimate the severity of their challenges, at least in the domain of executive functions.

Our study should be interpreted in the light of its limitations. The most important is potential selection bias. Outcomes of patients whose families agreed to participate might not be representative of outcomes among all adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot. In the absence of data on outcomes of non-participants, however, the direction of a bias introduced by selective enrollment is uncertain. Genetic testing was performed in most, but not in every patient. The patients’ surgeries occurred more than a decade ago, and more recent cohorts of patients with tetralogy of Fallot might have benefited from recent improvements in surgical and medical management. Although we found that a greater number of post-operative complications was associated with greater neurodevelopmental impairment, concordant with findings in other studies,Reference Newburger, Sleeper and Bellinger 29 the complications of catheterisation and surgery may have been collinear and are variable in severity.

In summary, although many adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot have neurodevelopmental outcomes within the average range, a sizeable minority do not, even among patients without a known genetic/phenotypic diagnosis. Our results support recent AHA/AAP guidelines, recommending enhanced neurodevelopmental surveillance of a child with congenital heart diseaseReference Marino, Lipkin and Newburger 30 so that appropriate, timely interventions can be tailored to assist the patient in achieving his or her fullest potential. Furthermore, our results suggest that such surveillance should extend beyond the age of 11–12 years, the oldest age for follow-up suggested in these guidelines.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Drs Lisa Farley, Gloria Chiang, and Lou Baptista-Nadeau, as well as Jonathan Hirshberg, Carrie Morris, and Sneha Rao, for assistance in data collection; Kayla Monzak, Jill Cotter, Kate Henry, Naomi Lawrence-Reid, and Elizabeth Coogan, who served as research assistants; Donna Duva and Donna Donati who assisted with scheduling and other study logistics; and Kathy Alexander who provided administrative support. They also thank the families for their time and interest in participating.

Financial Support

This research was funded by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (J.W.N., P50 HL74734) and by the Farb Family Fund.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

1. Majnemer, A, Limperopoulos, C. Developmental progress of children with congenital heart defects requiring open heart surgery. Sem Pediatr Neurol 1999; 6: 1219.Google Scholar
2. Loffredo, CA. Epidemiology of cardiovascular malformations: prevalence and risk factors. Am J Med Genet 2000; 97: 319325.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Bellinger, DC, Wypij, D, duPlessis, AJ, et al. Developmental and neurologic effects of alpha-stat versus pH-stat strategies for deep hypothermic circulatory arrest in infants. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2001; 121: 374383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4. Zeltser, I, Jarvik, GP, Bernbaum, J, et al. Genetic/phenotypic factors are important determinants of neurodevelopmental outcome after repair of tetralogy of Fallot. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2008; 135: 9197.Google Scholar
5. Gaynor, JW, Gerdes, M, Nord, AS, et al. Is cardiac diagnosis a predictor of neurodevelopmental outcome after cardiac surgery in infancy? J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2010; 140: 12301237.Google Scholar
6. Hovels-Gurich, HH, Konrad, K, Skorzenski, D, et al. Long-term neurodevelopmental outcome and exercise capacity after corrective surgery for tetralogy of Fallot or ventricular septal defect in infancy. Ann Thorac Surg 2006; 81: 958966.Google Scholar
7. Hovels-Gurich, HH, Konrad, K, Skorzenski, D, Herpertz-Dahlmann, B, Messmer, BJ, Seghaye, MC. Attentional dysfunction in children after corrective cardiac surgery in infancy. Ann Thorac Surg 2007; 83: 14251430.Google Scholar
8. Hovels-Gurich, HH, Bauer, SB, Schnitker, R, et al. Long-term outcome of speech and language in children after corrective surgery for cyanotic or acyanotic cardiac defects in infancy. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2008; 12: 378386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Miatton, M, De Wolf, D, François, K, Thiery, E, Vingerhoets, G. Intellectual, neuropsychological, and behavioral functioning in children with tetralogy of Fallot. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2007; 133: 449455.Google Scholar
10. Shampaine, EL, Nadelman, L, Rosenthal, A, Behrendt, D, Sloan, H. Longitudinal psychological assessment in tetralogy of Fallot. Pediatr Cardiol 1990; 10: 135150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11. Daliento, I, Mapelli, D, Russo, G, et al. Health related quality of life in adults with repaired tetralogy of Fallot: psychosocial and cognitive outcomes. Heart 2005; 91: 213218.Google Scholar
12. Baker, K, Vorstman, JA. Is there a core neuropsychiatric phenotype in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome? Current Opinion in Neurology 2012; 25: 131137.Google Scholar
13. Evans, AC, Brain Development Cooperative Group. The NIH MRI study of normal brain development. NeuroImage 2006; 30: 184202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Wechsler, D. WISC-IV. Administration and Scoring Manual. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, 2003.Google Scholar
15. The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-Second Edition. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, 2002.Google Scholar
16. Wechsler, D. WISC-IV Integrated Technical and Interpretive Manual. Harcourt Assessment Inc., San Antonio, 2004.Google Scholar
17. Cohen, MJ. Children’s Memory Scale. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, 1997.Google Scholar
18. Delis, DC, Kaplan, E, Kramer, JH. Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, 2001.Google Scholar
19. Gioia, GA, Isquith, PK, Guy, SC, Kenworthy, L. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL, 2000.Google Scholar
20. Guy, SC, Isquith, PK, Gioia, GA. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Self-Report Version. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Lutz, FL, 2004.Google Scholar
21. Gardner, MF. Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (Non-Motor) (Upper Level) Revised. Hydeville. Psychological and Educational Publications Inc, CA, 1997.Google Scholar
22. Bernstein, JH, Waber, DP. Developmental scoring system for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Lutz, FL, 1996.Google Scholar
23. Hegarty, M, Richardson, AE, Montello, DR, Lovelace, K, Subbiah, I. Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. Intelligence 2002; 30: 425447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24. Conners, CK. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised. Multi-Health Systems Inc, North Tonawanda, NY, 1997.Google Scholar
25. Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Hill, J, Raste, Y, Plumb, I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42: 241251.Google Scholar
26. Wheelwright, S, Auyeung, B, Allison, C, Baron-Cohen, S. Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Molec Autism 2010; 1: 110.Google Scholar
27. Bagby, RM, Parker, JDA, Taylor, GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38: 2332.Google Scholar
28. Bellinger, DC, Wypij, D, Rivkin, MJ, et al. Adolescents with d-transposition of the great arteries corrected with the arterial switch procedure. Circulation 2011; 124: 13611369.Google Scholar
29. Newburger, JW, Sleeper, LA, Bellinger, DC, et al. Early developmental outcome in children with hypoplastic left heart syndrome and related anomalies: the single ventricle reconstruction trial. Circulation 2012; 125: 20812091.Google Scholar
30. Marino, BS, Lipkin, PH, Newburger, JW, et al. American Heart Association Congenital Heart Defects Committee, Council on Cardiovascular Disease in the Young, Council on Cardiovascular Nursing, and Stroke Council. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in children with congenital heart disease: evaluation and management: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2012; 126: 11431172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot (n=91).

Figure 1

Table 2 Neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot according to genetic diagnosis.

Figure 2

Table 3 Structural magnetic resonance imaging findings of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot and referents.

Figure 3

Table 4 Stepwise linear regression of select neuropsychological outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot without genetic anomalies (n=68), adjusting for concurrent family social status.

Figure 4

Table 5 Stepwise logistic regression of select magnetic resonance imaging outcomes of adolescents with tetralogy of Fallot (n=66).